As shown by the Tuskegee Syphilis Study, the Stanford Prison Experiment and the Milgram Experiment, an informed consent policy is an essential part of any scientific research.
All three of these studies broke the rules in different ways.
In the Tuskegee Study, researchers did not inform the participants that they would be denied treatment, just one of the ways in which this disgraceful experiment failed.
The Stanford Prison Experiment and Milgram Experiment failed to assess the risks and instruct the participants about any potential psychological damage.
Deception can occur through concealment, where a subject is not told the whole truth about the potential effects, or deception, where the participant is deliberately lied to.
Psychological and medical experiments attempt to use controls, and this is where problems can arise.
A true control should use blind methods, but this is a difficult balance to strike. Very few researchers would deliberately put a subject at risk, but there is another problem.
The main problem is that most participants are non-scientists, and it is all too easy to inadvertently deceive them because they do not fully understand the consequences.
Here is a summary of the APA code of ethics and informed consent policy - most other research areas use similar codes of practice.
These have been set in stone for many years, but have recently been adapted. A subject signs an initial consent form but, after the experiment has been explained at the end, they sign a second and can ask for their contribution and records to be destroyed. Part of this is due to various Data Protection acts, but the rise of reality TV has also contributed to a strengthening of ethical controls and consent.
The 'Big Brother' TV series and many other alleged 'psychological experiments' provided an initial informed consent policy form, but it could be argued that the participants did not realize that the program is subjected to selective editing, portraying them in the worst possible light.
Under the adjusted guidelines, a contestant could ask for their contribution to be removed, which would jeopardize the whole filming process. Of course, there are many other reasons why these programs cannot be classed as a psychological experiment, but ethical concerns and consent are an issue that may hit the courtrooms soon.
Modern belief is that except in extreme circumstances, a subject should be informed about the dangers before the experiment. At the very least, they should have been given an informed consent policy at the end and have the opportunity to have any data about them destroyed.
In this example, Milgram's experiment is a grey area, because the subjects were informed straight after the study. If a researcher designed a similar experiment today, it would be unlikely to pass ethical considerations.
At the time of experimentation, when the world was trying to understand if 'Just obeying orders,' was a viable excuse for the worst excesses of the Nazi regime, then it could be argued that the usefulness of the results outweighed the distress caused.
The morale of this is that any experiment has to be judged upon a case-by-case basis. The Stanford Prison Experiment did have as much consent as possible, but the failing in this case was the failure of the researcher to halt the experiment when distress was observed.
For example, Zimbardo, the head researcher in the experiment, should have remained outside the study and pulled the plug, rather than allowing himself to be drawn in to the psychological morass.
The Tuskegee Study had no justification, and the researchers not only failed to inform, but also broke their medical commitment to the Hippocratic Oath, failing to preserve life. This is a much more serious issue than consent, and is no different from the notorious Nazi experiments.
No ethical code or informed consent policy can be perfect, and there will always be situations where the experimenter caused too much distress.
On the other hand, there may be times when the experiment lost some validity due to informed consent. This is a problem with no correct answer, although modern techniques err on the side of caution.
Very few peer reviewers will be too strict upon questioning the validity of experiment because of ethical caution.
Instead, reviewers understand that error arising from this has to be incorporated into the interpretation of the results. This is something to which science has to work around and adapt.
Martyn Shuttleworth (Mar 12, 2009). Informed Consent Policy. Retrieved Sep 14, 2024 from Explorable.com: https://explorable.com/informed-consent-policy
The text in this article is licensed under the Creative Commons-License Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0).
This means you're free to copy, share and adapt any parts (or all) of the text in the article, as long as you give appropriate credit and provide a link/reference to this page.
That is it. You don't need our permission to copy the article; just include a link/reference back to this page. You can use it freely (with some kind of link), and we're also okay with people reprinting in publications like books, blogs, newsletters, course-material, papers, wikipedia and presentations (with clear attribution).